Skip to main content
Log in

We Are Happier than We Realize: Underestimation and Conflation in Measuring Happiness

  • Research Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Happiness Studies Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Absract

The study evaluates a very common question designed to measure happiness: “Taken all together, how would you say things are these days–would you say that you are very happy, pretty happy, or not too happy?” Through five representative survey experiments, we show that (1) this survey item underestimates the level of happiness with one’s life; (2) this is because the measure is more likely to reflect satisfaction with the state of the world rather than personal life; (3) this measures is more susceptible to priming; (4) the addition of three words “in your life” to the item greatly reduces priming and question order effects; and (5) the addition of these three words produces results that are very similar to life satisfaction measures that include “in your life” and are more positively associated with income. These results provide evidence that a simple correction better measures personal happiness. Furthermore, our findings reassess the foundation of a considerable volume of scholarship about how politics and income is associated happiness.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1

Source: University Poll

Fig. 2

Source: Elon University Poll, Feb. 2013. N = 886

Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. http://www.federalreserve.gov/newsevents/speech/bernanke20100508a.htm---It should be noted that in this speech Bernanke specifically cites the survey item we evaluate in this study.

  2. The advantage of using external evaluations of happiness is that one can avoid problems stemming from common-source bias. Researchers have observed that when two self-reported measures are obtained from the same source, such as a single survey, the association between the two measures is often inflated (Favero and Bullock 2015; Meier and O’Toole 2010).

  3. Additionally, Smith (1979) investigated the slight difference between the National Opinion Research Center (NORC) wording and the Survey Research Center, University of Michigan (SRC) wording of a single item happiness question and finds noticeable differences in responses but suggests that much of this may be due to question ordering in the two surveys.

  4. GSS variable name HAPPY. A Google Scholar search of the exact question revealed 628 results. Document counts accessed on 9/19/2015.

  5. Mutz (2011) argues that “population-based survey experiments provide a means of establishing causality that is unmatched by any large-scale survey data collection effort, no matter how extensive.”

  6. Details of each survey is presented in Table 5 in the Online Supporting Materials.

  7. See Nicholson-Crotty and Meier (2002) for a defense of single-state studies.

  8. The results were also consistent across survey mode. In an online, web-based survey we found that the modified happiness question produced happier responses than the standard question wording.

  9. To assess accuracy of coding, we coded responses with multiple coders. Intercoder reliability was 0.71. Other scales could be used for this coding.

  10. We estimate a parsimonious model. However, results are consistent with additional right-hand side variables.

  11. A content analysis of the February 2013 open-ended responses to the questions asking why a respondent was happy or unhappy found that 8 percent of respondents mentioned "government" when "in your life" wasn't in the wording. This is compared to only 1 percent when "in your life" was included in question. In addition, only 3 respondents mentioned the president or his name in the "in your life" survey item, while 14 referenced the president when those words were dropped from the question.

  12. For example, the World Values Survey; European Social Survey, and European Quality of Life Survey use such a measure.

  13. Although Fry and Stutzer (2005) question the causal direction of this relationship and find that for some age groups happy people are more likely to get married.

  14. We replicated the analysis with each of the four telephone surveys containing the 4 category income measure and found in every case our modified happiness measure was more strongly correlated with income than the standard happiness measure.

  15. We estimate a parsimonious model. However, results are consistent with additional right-hand side variables.

References

  • Abdel-Khalek, A. M. (2006). Measuring happiness with a single-item scale. Social Behavior and Personality: An International Journal, 34(2), 139–150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alvarez-Diaz, A., González, L., & Radcliff, B. (2010). The politics of happiness: On the political determinants of quality of life in the American states. The Journal of Politics, 72(03), 894–905.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Andrews, F. M., & Robinson, J. P. (1991). Measures of subjective well-being. In J. P. Robinson, P. R. Shaver, & L. S. Wrightsman (Eds.), Measures of personality and social psychological attitudes (pp. 61–114). San Diego: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Brickman, P., Coates, D., & Janoff-Bulman, R. (1978). Lottery winners and accident victims: Is happiness relative? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36(8), 917.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheung, Felix, & Lucas, Richard E. (2014). Assessing the validity of single-item life satisfaction measures: results from three large samples. Quality of Life Research, 23(10), 2809–2818.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Connolly, M. (2013). Some like it mild and not too wet: The influence of weather on subjective well-being. Journal of Happiness Studies, 14(2), 457–473.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cummins, R. A. (1995). On the trail of the gold standard for subjective well-being. Social Indicators Research, 35(2), 179–200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Di Tella, R., & MacCulloch, R. (2005). Partisan social happiness. The Review of Economic Studies, 72(2), 367–393.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E. (2000). Subjective well-being: The science of happiness and a proposal for a national index. American Psychologist, 55(1), 34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., Inglehart, R., & Tay, L. (2012). Theory and validity of life satisfaction scales. Social Indicators Research, 112(3), 497–527.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Easterlin, R. A. (1995). Will raising the incomes of all increase the happiness of all? Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 27(1), 35–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Easterlin, R. A. (2013). Happiness, growth, and public policy†. Economic Inquiry, 51(1), 1–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Favero, N., & Bullock, J. B. (2015). How (not) to solve the problem: An evaluation of scholarly responses to common source bias. Journal of Public Administration and Theory, 25(1), 285–308.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferrer-i-Carbonell, A., & Frijters, P. (2004). How important is methodology for the estimates of the determinants of happiness? The Economic Journal, 114(497), 641–659.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flavin, P., Pacek, A., & Radcliff, B. (2014). Size of government and human well-being: Assessing the impact of the size and scope of government on human well-being. Social Forces, 92(4), 1241–1258.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fowler, F. J. (1995). Improving survey questions: Design and evaluation. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frey, B. S., & Stutzer, A. (2000). Happiness, economy and institutions. The Economic Journal, 110(466), 918–938.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frey, B. S., & Stutzer, A. (2002). What can economists learn from happiness research? Journal of Economic literature., 40, 402–435.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frey, B. S., & Stutzer, A. (2010). Happiness and public choice. Public Choice, 144(3–4), 557–573.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gerber, A. S., & Huber, G. A. (2010). Partisanship, political control, and economic assessments. American Journal of Political Science, 54(1), 153–173.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gerber, A. S., Huber, G. A., Doherty, D., & Dowling, C. M. (2013). Assessing the stability of psychological and political survey measures. American Politics Research, 41(1), 54–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graham, J. M. (2011). Measuring love in romantic relationships: A meta-analysis. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 28(6), 748–771.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graham, C. (2012). The pursuit of happiness: An economy of well-being. Washington, DC: Brookings Institution Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haller, M., & Hadler, M. (2006). How social relations and structures can produce happiness and unhappiness: An international comparative analysis. Social Indicators Research, 75(2), 169–216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Helliwell, J. F. (2003). How’s life? Combining individual and national variables to explain subjective well-being. Economic Modelling, 20(2), 331–360.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Helliwell, J. F., & Huang, H. (2008). How’s your government? International evidence linking good government and well-being. British Journal of Political Science, 38(04), 595–619.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hills, P., & Argyle, M. (2002). The Oxford Happiness Questionnaire: A compact scale for the measurement of psychological well-being. Personality and Individual Differences, 33(7), 1073–1082.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Howell, R. T., Rodzon, K. S., Kurai, M., & Sanchez, A. H. (2010). A validation of well-being and happiness surveys for administration via the Internet. Behavior Research Methods, 42(3), 775–784.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huang, P. H. (2010). Happiness studies and legal policy. Annual Review of Law and Social Science, 6, 405–432.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huppert, F. A. (2009). Psychological Well-being: Evidence Regarding its Causes and Consequences†. Applied Psychology: Health and Well-Being, 1(2), 137–164.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kalmijn, W. M., Arends, L. R., & Veenhoven, R. (2011). Happiness scale interval study. Methodological considerations. Social Indicators Research, 102(3), 497–515.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krueger, A. B., & Schkade, D. A. (2008). The reliability of subjective well-being measures. Journal of public economics, 92(8), 1833–1845.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lane, R. E. (2000). The loss of happiness in market democracies. CT, New Haven: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Layard, R. (2005). Happiness: Lessons from a New Science. London: Allen Lane.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lucas, R. E., & Donnellan, M. B. (2012). Estimating the reliability of single-item life satisfaction measures: Results from four national panel studies. Social Indicators Research, 105(3), 323–331.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lucas, Richard E., & Schimmack, Ulrich. (2009). Income and well-being: How big is the gap between the rich and the poor? Journal of Research in Personality, 43(1), 75–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lyubomirsky, S., & Lepper, H. S. (1999). A measure of subjective happiness: Preliminary reliability and construct validation. Social Indicators Research, 46(2), 137–155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacKerron, G. (2012). Happiness economics from 35,000 feet. Journal of Economic Surveys, 26(4), 705–735.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McClendon, M. J., & O’Brien, D. J. (1988). Question-order effects on the determinants of subjective well-being. Public Opinion Quarterly, 52(3), 351–364.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meier, K. J., & O’Toole, L. J., Jr. (2010). Subjective organizational performance and measurement error: common source bias and spurious relationships. Journal of Public Administration Research and Theory, 23(2), 429–445.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moskowitz, D. S. (1986). Comparison of self-reports, reports by knowledgeable informants, and behavioral observation data. Journal of Personality, 54(1), 294–317.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mutz, D. C. (2011). Population-based survey experiments. Pinceton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Nicholson-Crotty, S., & Meier, K. J. (2002). Size doesn’t matter: In defense of single-state studies. State Politics & Policy Quarterly, 2(4), 411–422.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Orviska, M., Caplanova, A., & Hudson, J. (2014). The impact of democracy on well-being. Social Indicators Research, 115(1), 493–508.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oswald, A. J. (1997). Happiness and economic performance. The Economic Journal, 107(445), 1815–1831.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pacek, A., & Radcliff, B. (2008a). Assessing the welfare state: The politics of happiness. Perspectives on Politics, 6(02), 267–277.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pacek, A. C., & Radcliff, B. (2008b). Welfare policy and subjective well-being across nations: An individual-level assessment. Social Indicators Research, 89(1), 179–191.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pavot, W., & Diener, E. (1993). Review of the satisfaction with life scale. Psychological Assessment, 5(2), 164.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Porta, P. L., & Scazzieri, R. (2007). 5 Public happiness and civil society. In L. Bruni & P. L. Porta (Eds.), Handbook on the economics of happiness (pp. 95–109). Northhampton, MA: Edward Elgar.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rehdanz, K., & Maddison, D. (2005). Climate and happiness. Ecological Economics, 52(1), 111–125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rodríguez-Pose, A., & Maslauskaite, K. (2012). Can policy make us happier? Individual characteristics, socio-economic factors and life satisfaction in Central and Eastern Europe. Cambridge Journal of Regions, Economy and Society, 5(1), 77–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ross, M., Eyman, A., & Kishchuk, N. (1986). Determinants of subjective well-being. In J. M. Olson, C. P. Herman, & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), Relative deprivation and social comparison: The Ontario symposium (pp. 79–93). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Røysamb, E., Nes, R. B., & Vittersø, J. (2014). Well-being: Heritable and changeable. In K. M. Sheldon & R. E. Lucas (Eds.), Stability of happiness: Theories and evidence on whether happiness Can change (pp. 9–36). London: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Ryff, C. D. (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the meaning of psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57(6), 1069.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sandvik, E., Diener, E., & Seidlitz, L. (1993). Subjective well-being: The convergence and stability of self-report and non-self-report measures. Journal of Personality, 61(3), 317–342.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schimmack, U., Diener, E., & Oishi, S. (2002). Life-satisfaction is a momentary judgment and a stable personality characteristic: The use of chronically accessible and stable sources. Journal of Personality, 70(3), 345–384.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schlenker, B. R., Chambers, J. R., & Le, B. M. (2012). Conservatives are happier than liberals, but why? Political ideology, personality, and life satisfaction. Journal of Research in Personality, 46(2), 127–146.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N., & Strack, F. (1991). Evaluating one’s life: A judgment model of subjective well-being. Subjective well-being: An interdisciplinary perspective, 21, 27–47.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N., & Strack, F. (1999). Reports of subjective well-being: Judgmental processes and their methodological implications. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N., Strack, F., & Mai, H. P. (1991). Assimilation and contrast effects in part-whole question sequences: A conversational logic analysis. Public Opinion Quarterly, 55(1), 3–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scott, W. A. (1958). Research definitions of mental health and mental illness. Psychological Bulletin, 55(1), 29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, T. W. (1979). Happiness: Time trends, seasonal variations, intersurvey differences, and other mysteries. Social Psychology Quarterly, 42(1), 18–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, T. W. (1990). Timely artifacts: A review of measurement variation in the 1972-1989 GSS. GSS Methodological Report 56.

  • Stones, M. J., Hadjistavropoulos, T., Tuuko, H., & Kozma, A. (1995). Happiness has traitlike and statelike properties: A reply to Veenhoven. Social Indicators Research, 36(2), 129–144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Strack, F., Martin, L. L., & Schwarz, N. (1988). Priming and communication: Social determinants of information use in judgments of life satisfaction. European journal of social psychology, 18(5), 429–442.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Studer, R. (2012). Does it matter how happiness is measured? Evidence from a randomized controlled experiment. Journal of Economic and Social Measurement, 37(4), 317–336.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tavits, M. (2008). Representation, corruption, and subjective well-being. Comparative Political Studies, 41(12), 1607–1630.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tourangeau, R., Rasinski, K. A., & Bradburn, N. (1991). Measuring happiness in surveys: A test of the subtraction hypothesis. Public Opinion Quarterly, 55(2), 255–266.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Praag, B. M., et al. (2003). The anatomy of subjective well-being. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 51(1), 29–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Veenhoven, R. (1991). Is happiness relative? Social Indicators Research, 24(1), 1–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Veenhoven, R. (1995). The cross-national pattern of happiness: Test of predictions implied in three theories of happiness. Social Indicators Research, 34(1), 33–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wanous, J. P., & Reichers, A. E. (1996). Estimating the reliability of a single-item measure. Psychological Reports, 78(2), 631–634.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weiss, A., et al. (2008). Happiness is a personal (ity) thing: The genetics of personality and well-being in a representative sample. Psychological Science, 19(3), 205–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weitz-Shapiro, R., & Winters, M. S. (2011). The link between voting and life satisfaction in Latin America. Latin American Politics and Society, 53(4), 101–126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wills-Herrera, E., Orozco, L. E., Forero-Pineda, C., Pardo, O., & Andonova, V. (2011). The relationship between perceptions of insecurity, social capital and subjective well-being: Empirical evidences from areas of rural conflict in Colombia. The Journal of Socio-Economics, 40(1), 88–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jason A. Husser.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary material 1 (DOCX 17 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Husser, J.A., Fernandez, K.E. We Are Happier than We Realize: Underestimation and Conflation in Measuring Happiness. J Happiness Stud 19, 587–606 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-016-9831-0

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-016-9831-0

Keywords

Navigation