gms | German Medical Science

GMS Journal for Medical Education

Gesellschaft für Medizinische Ausbildung (GMA)

ISSN 2366-5017

Statistical literacy and scientific reasoning & argumentation in physicians

article statistical literacy

  • author Felicitas M. Schmidt - University Hospital, LMU Munich, Institute of Medical Education, Munich, Germany
  • author Jan M. Zottmann - University Hospital, LMU Munich, Institute of Medical Education, Munich, Germany
  • author Maximilian Sailer - University of Passau, Chair of Educational Science, Passau, Germany
  • author Martin R. Fischer - University Hospital, LMU Munich, Institute of Medical Education, Munich, Germany
  • corresponding author Markus Berndt - University Hospital, LMU Munich, Institute of Medical Education, Munich, Germany; Walden University, Richard W. Riley College of Education and Leadership, Minneapolis, USA

GMS J Med Educ 2021;38(4):Doc77

doi: 10.3205/zma001473, urn:nbn:de:0183-zma0014734

This is the English version of the article.
The German version can be found at: http://www.egms.de/de/journals/zma/2021-38/zma001473.shtml

Received: April 27, 2020
Revised: December 5, 2020
Accepted: January 25, 2021
Published: April 15, 2021

© 2021 Schmidt et al.
This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 License. See license information at http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.


Abstract

Objective: Statistical literacy (SL) of physicians, i.e. the ability to use and interpret statistical numbers in the context of science, is an essential prerequisite for risk estimation and communication. Together with scientific reasoning and argumentation (SRA) skills, SL provides the basis for evidence-based practice. Several studies suggest that in medical students both skills are underdeveloped. The aim of the present study was to investigate these skills in practicing physicians and how these skills were acquired.

Methods: Data collection in N=71 physicians was conducted online and as paper pencil. SL was assessed with multiple-choice items. SRA skills evidence evaluation and drawing conclusions were measured with a decision scenario.

Results: Study results indicated that physicians have medium levels of SL (M=17.58, SD=6.92, max 30 pts.) and SRA (evidence evaluation: M=7.75, SD=1.85, max 10 pts.; drawing conclusions: M=37.20, SD=5.35, max 60 pts.). Skills development via autodidactic learning activities (M=4.78, SD=1.13, range 1-6) was reported significantly more often than development during formal medical education (M=2.31, SD=1.46), t(71)=-9.915, p<.001, or in extracurricular activities (M=3.34, SD=1.87), t(71)=4.673, p<.001. The active involvement in research seemed decisive: The number of publications and time spent in research significantly correlated with SL, r(71)=.355, p=.002; respectively r(71)=.280, p=.018. SRA skills were predicted by the type of MD-thesis, β=-.380, p=.016, and working in research, β=3.355, p=.008.

Conclusion: Active involvement in research activities seems to be a very important factor for the development of both SL and SRA skills. The implementation of systematic fostering of these skills during formal medical education seems warranted.

Keywords: evidence-based practice, scientific reasoning and argumentation, statistical literacy, medical education, postgraduate medical education


1. Background

Following Sackett et al. (1997) regarding the modern definition of evidence-based medicine, scientific reasoning skills are considered essential together with physicians’ expertise for best possible decision-making in the best interest of the patient [1], [2], [3]. For the assessment of risks and probabilities and their applicability to specific patients, a basic understanding of statistics and evidence evaluation is necessary. Statistical literacy (SL) is not only the ability to understand statistical information, but also to apply it in decision-making [4]. It comprises the aptitude of critical reflection about statistics as evidence in arguments [5]. Statistical literacy is based on numeracy, the aptitude of mathematical operations [6], and encompasses the ability to use and interpret statistical numbers in the context of science [7], [8], [9] and the ability to explain and critically evaluate them [10], [11] (working definition for the present study). Furthermore, it is intertwined with scientific reasoning and argumentation skills (SRA), to provide the basis for evidence-based decision-making [12], [13].

Based on the framework by Fischer et al. (2014), SRA can be defined as the competence of comprehending and applying scientific working methods and their results when solving problems [14], [15]. SRA can be described by eight epistemic activities and this study focuses on two of them, evidence evaluation (EE) and drawing conclusions (DC).

Various studies suggest an intertwining of SRA and SL, with Anderson et al. (2013) stating that the latter is needed to evaluate scientific evidence [16] and Franklin et al. (2005) hypothesizing that SL itself encompasses also SRA skills [17].

However, a collective statistical illiteracy has been observed among physicians [4], [18]. Similarly, SRA skills needed for evidence-based practice [19], such as EE or DC, are underdeveloped [4], [7].

In a study by Anderson et al. (2014), 52% of the participating physicians answered only two (or fewer) of four questions regarding statistical concepts correctly [7]. This is in line with findings by Windish et al. (2007) reporting only 40% of resident physicians demonstrating adequate understanding of biostatistical concepts [20]. Similar results were found by Gigerenzer and Wegwarth (2008), showing that 33% of gynecologists were not aware of the benefits of mammography screening, with 79% being unable to interpret the positive predictive value [21]. Gigerenzer et al. (2008) summarized various studies on the concept of the positive predictive value and its dependence on prevalence, finding that 50% of participants were under the impression that false positive test results in HIV testing do not exist. They also found that only two of 20 urologists have sufficient knowledge about the reliability of a PSA-test. Thus, a lot of measurement tools for the assessment of SL exist [18], [22], [23], which focus commonly on one of the three levels of Watson (1997) or are designed in a certain context [7], [22]. Overall, physicians’ SL is not below-average [7]. However, it can be considered comparable to other educated samples [18], [24] and was found superior to that of residents in research training [20] or medical students [25]. The few studies that have assessed medical students’ SL are supporting the findings of superiority of physicians [26]. Berndt et al. (2021) compared medical students to those of social sciences and economics and found that medical students in their first years of study scored higher in comparison to social science students and comparable to economics students [27]. This study not only assessed SL, but also the two aforementioned skills EE and DC. Further possible links between SL and SRA have been examined with the Medical Data Interpretation Test [22] where participating physicians scored higher overall than participants with other postgraduate degrees (89 out of 100 score points). Johnson et al. (2014) assessed numeracy of medical students and residents and found students with poor numeracy being more likely to misjudge risks of different treatment alternatives and that the confidence in treatment recommendation increased during medical school [25].

In summary, SL and SRA seem to be underdeveloped in medical students and better developed in physicians. However, the development of SL and SRA may not necessarily happen within formal education and the question remains how, where, and when this development occurs. The present study should contribute to the discussion when and how to best foster SL and SRA skills in lifelong learning of physicians by applying a test instrument comprising various aspects of SL and SRA. It aimed at providing further insights into these skills in physicians and to identify demographic factors and learning opportunities that may be associated with the development of these skills. Our explorative research questions were:

RQ1. Statistical literacy and SRA skills

a. To what extent are SL and SRA skills developed in physicians?
b. To what extent does SL predict SRA skills of physicians?

RQ2. Education and skills development

a. How, where, and when do physicians develop SL and SRA skills?
b. Which demographic factors are related to the development of SL and SRA skills?

2. Methods

2.1. Design and sample

Our study followed a quasi-experimental, causal-comparative design with two dependent variables: SL and SRA. We included N=71 German-speaking physicians (31 females, 34 males, 6 NA), from different work settings and locations in our study: hospital (n=44), outpatient sector (n=3), research (n=8), study program (n=2) and administration (n=2), (n=12 NA). A MD-thesis, a scientific work as optional part of the medical study program (not equivalent to a PhD thesis), was completed by 58 participants and 9 were currently working on it. Despite the modest sample size, we consider our sample representative with regard to scientific experience, as a MD-thesis is very common in Germany. The mean age of participants was 40 years (SD=9.59, range=26-65) (see table 1 [Tab. 1]).

2.2. Test instrument

For the assessment of SL and SRA skills, we used an instrument previously developed in the context of a study by Berndt et al. (2021) who initiated the ongoing validation process with 217 economics, social sciences, and medical students from LMU Munich [27]. The test instrument combines multiple choice items to assess SL with a decision scenario [28] to assess the participants’ skills in EE and DC. For this study, items on relevant demographic factors were added and piloted with ten medical students from LMU Munich.

2.2.1. Demography

Demographic and biographic parameters of the participants were assessed with a special interest in their working history and environment (hospital, out-patient care, research). Questions were adapted from a study by Epstein et al. [29] and comprised multiple choice items, some with the opportunity to fill in additional free text; five items on the MD-thesis, three items on the professional career, two items on the publication record (type of authorship, number of publications), and three items on the current job description.

2.2.2. Statistical literacy

Statistical literacy was measured with multiple choice items based on validated instruments [7], [18], [23], to assess a broad spectrum from basic numeracy to conditional probabilities and statistical concepts. Duplicates and factual knowledge questions were excluded, so that all three levels described by Watson (1997) were covered. Additionally, all items were weighted for difficulty [11]. Internal consistency of the SL test was .82 (Cronbach’s α) in our sample with a maximum score of 30 points. All items were framed in a medical context; however, no medical content knowledge was necessary to answer them correctly.

2.2.3. SRA skills

The assessment of SRA skills focused on the two epistemic activities EE and DC with a decision scenario in a medical context (general medicine, out-patient care) and provided two separate overall scores for EE and DC (Cronbach's α for EE items .87; for DC items .74). For EE, participants had to evaluate four pieces of evidence [30], [31], [32], including one authentic pharmaceutical brochure that advertised herbal drugs, in terms of their scientific quality, evidence strength, and relevance for the present situation on a 6-point Likert scale based on the QUESTS criteria [33].

Then, the participants rated the persuasiveness (Likert 1-6) of 20 arguments, which were extracted from the presented evidence and assigned a level of argument strength from 1 (lowest) to 4 (highest). For 13 participants, the evaluation of 1 to 5 arguments out of 20 was missing. In order to avoid dropouts of these cases, the respective values were imputed from the average of the respective item. The participants’ ratings for scientific quality were compared to the ratings of scientific quality by the authors, resulting in a measure of similarity for EE and DC. The range of these measures was from 0-10 (EE score) and 0-60 (DC score) with zero indicating no similarity.

2.3. Procedure

The study was completed by the participants either online with LamaPoll [https://www.lamapoll.de/], a survey tool optimized for mobile applications, or as paper pencil (return rate 16.5% online and 66.7% paper pencil). Average duration was approximately 45 minutes. Participants were invited via mailing lists and personal contacts.

2.4. Statistical analyses

Statistical analyses were performed with IBM SPSS 25. Descriptive and frequency data were computed for primary analysis and Cronbach’s alpha for internal consistency. Extensive outlier analyses were conducted and all required prerequisites for statistical analyses, such as normal distribution and homoscedasticity, were tested. T-tests, one-factorial ANOVAs, and linear regression models were calculated to assess differences and the association of demographic factors with SL and SRA. Probability values less than .05 were considered significant. Data in natural verbal language (free text in demography section) underwent independent thematic analysis by two authors to extract common themes.


3. Results

We included 71 completed questionnaires (see table 1 [Tab. 1]). The entire data set was checked for univariate outliers. Skewness and kurtosis for all variables was within the ±2 range [34]. The prerequisites for t-tests and ANOVA were fulfilled, unless indicated otherwise below.

3.1. Statistical literacy and SRA skills

The 71 physicians' average score in SL was M=17.58, SD=6.92, with a range of 5 to 30 out of 30 attainable points (59%). On average, physicians evaluated the evidences concordantly with the authors' evaluation, EE score: M=7.75, SD=1.85 (77%). The ratings for argument quality were in accordance with the authors’ rating, DC score: M=37.20, SD=5.35 (62%). Statistical literacy and DC were significantly inversely correlated, r(71)=-.272, p=.022. However, no correlation was found between SL and EE, r(71)=.198, p=.098, nor EE and DC, r(71)=.138, p=.256.

3.2. Education and skills development

We explored how, where, and when physicians developed scientific skills (see figure 1 [Fig. 1]). Significantly more participants indicated to have acquired scientific skills in an autodidactic manner (M=4.78, SD=1.13, Likert 1-6 scale) rather than during their study program (M=2.31, SD=1.46, Likert 1-6 scale), t(71)=-9.915, p<.001, or in extracurricular activities (M=3.34, SD=1.87, Likert 1-6 scale), t(71)=4.673, p<.001. In a free-text box, participants added various other learning opportunities, such as massive open online courses, higher education, workshops, and learning through peer reviews and feedback (see figure 2 [Fig. 2]).

Having completed or working on the MD-thesis showed no effects on SL, EE, or DC. However, these results have to be treated carefully, as the prerequisites for ANOVA were not fulfilled in our sample with only four participants without a MD-thesis. The fostering of critical scrutiny of study results presented by other researchers during the preparation of the MD-thesis was positively correlated with SL, r(71)=.271, p=.033.

Regarding the postgraduate phase, a one-factorial ANOVA showed a significant main effect of having worked in research on SL, F(1,70)=12.737, p=.001, partial η2=.156 and the type of authorship in publications, F(5,71)=3.886, p=.004, partial η2=.230.

Time spent in research was significantly associated with better SL, r(71)=.28, p=.018, as was the number of publications, r(71)=.36, p=.002.

Regarding SRA, linear regression models revealed that the corresponding score in EE increased by β=.314±.150, p=.041, when the Likert value of the MD-thesis supervisor’s content-related support was increased by one point. Additionally, the form of the MD-thesis (experimental, clinical, empirical, statistical, or literature review) was associated with EE, with experimental and clinical design being positively related to EE skills, β=-.380±.154, p=.016, R2=.187, F(1,59)=4.353, p=.041. DC was higher when participants indicated to have already worked in research, β=3.355±1.229, p=.008, R2=.314 F(1,68)=7.448, p=.008.


4. Discussion

4.1. Research question 1: Statistical literacy and SRA skills

We found average statistical literacy of physicians (59%), a rather high-level EE score (77%) and a medium-level DC score (62%). SL did not predict the SRA skills of physicians.

Due to the focus on SL rather than the combination of basic numeracy and SL [27], our test instrument discriminated well, and we did not find any ceiling effects as observed in other educated samples [7], [35]. A comparison to other studies assessing SL of physicians is not easily done as every test covers a different range of SL. Schmidt et al. (2017) focused on knowledge of 18 different statistical tests among pathologists and observed a rather low level of SL [36]. Anderson et al. (2014) did not create an overall score but distinguished between fact, concept, and relation questions and found altering levels of SL [7]. A study with Greek residents also concentrated on knowledge questions and reported a rather low SL [26]. The EE and DC scores of German medical students we had previously examined with a similar instrument [27] were almost on the same level as the physicians’ scores in the present study. Riegelman and Hoveland (2012) found that residents struggled when critical reflection upon research was required [37], whereas the physicians in our study showed medium to high levels of SRA skills.

EE and SL scores were not correlated. DC and SL were inversely correlated. In contextual frameworks, SL has been regarded as a prerequisite for SRA [38] and in a Dutch community-based study, more numerate participants showed enhanced performance in SRA due to increased evaluation of pros and cons in decision-making and evaluation of judgments [39]. As evidence has not been predominantly presented in numerical or statistical terms, the missing link of EE and SL was expected, but the antithetical relationship of DC and SL was not. Future research could incorporate statistical information in decision scenarios in order to further analyze this connection in practicing physicians.

4.2. Research question 2: Education and skills development

We explored how, where, and when physicians developed SL and SRA skills. They indicated to have acquired scientific skills mostly in an autodidactic manner, in higher education outside of their medical study program, or in extracurricular activities.

Better SL was associated with the fostering of critical scrutiny of study results during the time spent working on the MD-thesis, in research or having worked in research, the number of publications, and the type of authorship. Our findings are in line with Schmidt et al. (2017), who found that having an advanced degree other than MD or statistic courses were positively associated with SL. A study with physicians, residents, and final year medical students in Thailand showed – not surprisingly – that having recently completed a statistical workshop led to higher SL scores [40]. However, additional courses are often hard to integrate in medical training. A study showed that 37% of American Ob-Gyn residents do not receive formal training [16], while another study with neurology residents observed a lack of acceptance for interventions on SL [41].

In the present study, better EE was associated with having been responsible for a research project (e.g. the MD-thesis) with experimental or clinical design and having content-related support by the supervisor. These findings are in line with the subjective impression of German medical graduates with a MD-thesis who rated their scientific skills higher compared to those working on it [29]. However, the participants in the study by Epstein et al. (2018) did not feel confident enough to conduct research on their own. This is particularly important because having already worked in research was associated with a higher SL and DC score in the present study and in Schmidt et al. (2017). Moreover, Epstein et al. (2018) found that medical graduates self-estimate their scientific skills after medical school as rather low. In the United States, only 68.1% of medical students in their final year participated in research during medical school and only 42% had (co-)authored a paper submitted for publication. It seems important that medical students become involved in research projects and the subsequent publication of findings during the completion of their MD-thesis, as this might enhance their SL and SRA skills in the long run.

4.3. Strengths and limitations

This study built upon an innovative approach by the authors to assess SL and SRA skills in university students [27]. The inclusive approach of SL assessment allowed a better description of the actual skills. However, it comes with the disadvantage of limited comparability with prior research. As the participant group of practicing physicians is not easily recruited, we considered the sample size of N=71 to be satisfactory. While generalizability is potentially limited, our sample seems representative for the German-speaking medical education system which produces large numbers of medical doctoral degrees. The addition of numerous demographic variables yielded insights on how, where, and when scientific skills were acquired and helped to identify potential associated factors.

Due to the broad age range in our sample, participants may have been exposed to different learning experiences in formal medical training and, depending on the place of study, may also have studied in reformed curricula. This could potentially have influenced their skills development and lead to further individual differences. In our study, we did not gather data on specific study programs, courses, and their descriptions, in which physicians might have acquired their skills. Already, the test instrument used in this study may be considered extensive and time consuming for physicians, as was indicated by 11 participants in their feedback.


5. Conclusion

We assessed SL and SRA skills in German-speaking physicians together with a thorough analysis of demographic variables. The active involvement in research apparently plays an important role in the development of these skills and might in consequence enhance evidence-based practice. As most participants indicated to have acquired these skills post-graduate and in an autodidactic manner, we argue to formalize and intensify the acquisition of these skills in medical study programs. Medical education curricula should include more statistical training and aim to get students involved in research more often, e.g. by offering inquiry-based learning [42] where students conduct research projects independently and are fully responsible for all phases of the research process.


Funding

This work was supported by the German Federal Ministry of Education and Research (BMBF, grant no. 01PB14004C) and an intramural grant of the Förderverein WiFoMed of the Medical Faculty of LMU Munich.


Acknowledgements

The authors would also like to thank Wolfgang Gaissmaier for his valuable comments and suggestions to the present study.


Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.


References

1.
Reyna VF, Nelson WL, Han PK, Dieckmann NF. How numeracy influences risk comprehension and medical decision making. Psychol Bull. 2009;135(6):943-973. DOI: 10.1037/a0017327 External link
2.
Nelson W, Reyna VF, Fagerlin A, Lipkus I, Peters E. Clinical implications of numeracy: theory and practice. Ann Behav Med. 2008;35(3):261-274. DOI: 10.1007/s12160-008-9037-8 External link
3.
Sackett DL, Rosenberg WM, Gray JA, Haynes RB, Richardson WS. Was ist Evidenz-basierte Medizin und was nicht? Münch Med Wochenschr. 1997;139(44):28-29. DOI: 10.1055/b-0036-140841 External link
4.
Gigerenzer G, Gaissmaier W, Kurz-Milcke E, Schwartz LM, Woloshin S. Helping Doctors and Patients Make Sense of Health Statistics. Psychol Sci Public Interest. 2008;8(2):53-96. DOI: 10.1111/j.1539-6053.2008.00033.x External link
5.
Shield M. Statistical literacy: thinking critically about statistics. Significance. 1999;1(1):15-20.
6.
Peters E. Beyond Comprehension: The Role of Numeracy in Judgments and Decisions. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2012;21(1):31-35. DOI: 10.1177/0963721411429960 External link
7.
Anderson BL, Gigerenzer G, Parker S, Schulkin J. Statistical literacy in obstetricians and gynecologists. J Healthc Qual. 2014;36(1):5-17. DOI: 10.1111/j.1945-1474.2011.00194.x External link
8.
Walker HM. Statistical literacy on the social science. Am Stat. 1951;5(1):6-12. DOI: 10.1080/00031305.1951.10481912 External link
9.
Wallman KK. Enhancing statistical literacy: enriching our society. J Am Statl Ass. 1993;88(421):1-8. DOI: 10.1080/01621459.1993.10594283 External link
10.
Ben-Zvi D, Garfield J. Statistical literacy, reasoning, and thinking: Goals, definitions, and challenges. In: Ben-Zvi D, Garfield J, editors. The challenge of developing statistical literacy, reasoning and thinking. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer; 2004. p.3-25. DOI: 10.1007/1-4020-2278-6 External link
11.
Watson JM. Assessing Statistical Thinking Using the Media. In: Gal I, Garfield JB, editors. The Assessment Challenge in Statistics Education. Amsterdam: IOS Press and The International Statistical Institute; 1997. p.107-121.
12.
Sedlmeier P, Gigerenzer G. Teaching Bayesian Reasoning in Less Than Two Hours. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2001;130(3):380-400. DOI: 10.1037/0096-3445.130.3.380 External link
13.
Fischer F, Kollar I, Ufer S, Sodian B, Hussmann H, Pekrun R, Neuhaus B, Dorner B, Pankofer S, Fischer M, Strijbos JW, Heene M, Eberle J. Scientific Reasoning and Argumentation: Advancing an Interdisciplinary Research Agenda in Education. Front Learn Res. 2014;2(3):28-45. DOI: 10.14786/flr.v2i2.96 External link
14.
Hetmanek A, Engelmann K, Opitz A, Fischer F. Beyond intelligence and domain knowledge: Scientific reasoning and argumentation as a set of cross-domain skills. In: Fischer F, Chinn CA, Engelmann K, Osborne J, editors. Scientific reasoning and argumentation: The roles of domain-specific and domain-general knowledge. New York: Routledge; 2018. p.203-226.
15.
Rudolph JL, Horibe S. What do we mean by science education for civic engagement? J Res Sci Teach. 2016;53(6):805-820. DOI: 10.1002/tea.21303 External link
16.
Anderson BL, Williams S, Schulkin J. Statistical literacy of obstetrics-gynecology residents. J Grad Med Educ. 2013;5(2):272-275. DOI: 10.4300/JGME-D-12-00161.1 External link
17.
Franklin C, Kader G, Mewborn D, Moreno J, Peck, R, Perry M, Schaeffler R. Guidelines for Assessment and Instruction in Statistics Education (GAISE) report: A Pre-K-12 curriculum framework. Alexandria: VA: American Statistical Association; 2005.
18.
Lipkus IM, Samsa G, Rimer BK. General performance on a numeracy scale among highly educated samples. Med Decis Making. 2001;21(1):37-44. DOI: 10.1177/0272989X0102100105 External link
19.
Monahan J. Statistical literacy: A prerequisite for evidence-based medicine. Los Angeles, CA: SAGE Publications Sage; 2007.
20.
Windish DM, Huot SJ, Green ML. Medicine residents' understanding of the biostatistics and results in the medical literature. JAMA. 2007;298(9):1010-1022. DOI: 10.1001/jama.298.9.1010 External link
21.
Gigerenzer G, Wegwarth O. Risikoabschätzung in der Medizin am Beispiel der Krebsfrüherkennung. Z Evid Fortbild Qual Gesundheitswes. 2008;102(9):513-519. DOI: 10.1016/j.zefq.2008.09.008 External link
22.
Schwartz LM, Woloshin S, Welch HG. Can patients interpret health information? An assessment of the medical data interpretation test. Med Decis Making. 2005;25(3):290-300. DOI: 10.1177/0272989X05276860 External link
23.
Cokely ET, Galesic M, Schulz E, Ghazal S, Garcia-Retamero R. Measuring Risk Literacy: The Berlin Numeracy Test. Judgment Dec Making. 2012;7(1):25-47. DOI: 10.1037/t45862-000 External link
24.
Okamoto M, Kyutoku Y, Sawada M, Clowney L, Watanabe E, Dan I, Kawamoto K. Health numeracy in Japan: measures of basic numeracy account for framing bias in a highly numerate population. BMC Med Inform Decis Mak. 2012;12:104. DOI: 10.1186/1472-6947-12-104 External link
25.
Johnson TV, Abbasi A, Schoenberg ED, Kellum R, Speake LD, Spiker C, Foust A, Kreps A, Ritenour CW, Brawleya OW, Master VA. Numeracy among trainees: are we preparing physicians for evidence-based medicine? J Surg Educ. 2014;71(2):211-215. DOI: 10.1016/j.jsurg.2013.07.013 External link
26.
Msaouel P, Kappos T, Tasoulis A, Apostolopoulos AP, Lekkas I, Tripodaki ES, Keramaris NC. Assessment of cognitive biases and biostatistics knowledge of medical residents: a multicenter, cross-sectional questionnaire study. Med Educ Online. 2014;19:23646. DOI: 10.3402/meo.v19.23646 External link
27.
Berndt M, Schmidt FM, Sailer M, Fischer F, Fischer MR, Zottmann JM. Investigating Statistical Literacy and Scientific Reasoning & Argumentation in Medical-, Social Sciences-, and Economics Students. Lern Individual Diff. 2021;86:101963. DOI: 10.1016/j.lindif.2020.101963 External link
28.
Trempler K, Hetmanek A, Wecker C, Kiesewetter J, Wermelt M, Fischer F, Fischer M, Gräsel C. Nutzung von Evidenz im Bildungsbereich - Validierung eines Instruments zur Erfassung von Kompetenzen der Informationsauswahl und der Bewertung von Studien. Z Pädagogik. 2015;61:144-166.
29.
Epstein N, Huber J, Gartmeier M, Berberat P, Reimer M, Fischer M. Investigating on the acquisition of scientific competencies during medical studies and the medical doctoral thesis. GMS J Med Educ. 2018;35(2):Doc20. DOI: 10.3205/zma001167 External link
30.
Simon D. Chronische Herzinsuffizienz. Apothek Umschau. 2016. Zugänglich unter/available from: http://www.apotheken.de/gesundheit-heute-news/article/chronische-herzinsuffizienz/ External link
31.
Neue Empfehlungen zum Einsatz von Weißdornpräparaten bei Herzinsuffizienz. Ärzteblatt.de. 2017. Zugänglich unter/available from: https://www.aerzteblatt.de/fachgebiete/kardiologie/news?nid=72259 External link
32.
Meissner T. Weißdorn: Effekt auf das Endothel im Fokus. ÄrzteZeitung. 2017.
33.
Harden M, Grant J, Buckley G, Hart R. BEME guide no. 1: best evidence medical education. Med Teach. 1999;21(6):553-526. DOI: 10.1080/01421599978960 External link
34.
Tabachnick BG, Fidell LS. Using multivariate statistics. 4th ed. Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon; 2001.
35.
Hanoch Y, Miron-Shatz T, Cole H, Himmelstein M, Federman AD. Choice, numeracy and physician-in-training performance: The case of Medicare part D. Health Psychol. 2010;29:454-459. DOI: 10.1037/a0019881 External link
36.
Schmidt RL, Chute DJ, Colbert-Getz JM, Firpo-Betancourt A, James DS, Karp JK, Miller DC, Milner DA Jr, Smock KJ, Sutton AT, Walker BS, White KL, Wilson AR, Wojcik EM, Yared MA, Factor RE. Statistical Literacy Among Academic Pathologists: A Survey Study to Gauge Knowledge of Frequently Used Statistical Tests Among Trainees and Faculty. Arch Pathol Lab Med. 2017;141(2):279-287. DOI: 10.5858/arpa.2016-0200-OA External link
37.
Riegelman RK, Hovland K. Scientific Thinking and Integrative Reasoning Skills (STIRS): Essential outcomes for medical education and for liberal education. Peer Rev. 2012;14(4):10.
38.
Watson JM, Callingham R. Statistical literacy: A complex hierarchical construct. Stat Educ Res J. 2003;2(2):3-46.
39.
Ghazal S, Cokely ET, Garcia-Retamero R. Predicting biases in very highly educated samples: Numeracy and metacognition. Judgment Dec Making. 2014;9(1):15-34.
40.
Laopaiboon M, Lumbiganon P, Walter SD. Doctors' statistical literacy: a survey at Srinagarind Hospital, Khon Kaen University. J Med Ass Thailand. 1997;80(2):130-137.
41.
Leira E, Granner M, Torner J, Callison R, Adams H. Education research: the challenge of incorporating formal research methodology training in a neurology residency. Neurology. 2008;70(20):e79-e84. DOI: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000312281.64033.36 External link
42.
Mieg H, editor. Inquiry-Based Learning - Undergraduate Research. Cham: Springer Nature; 2019. DOI: 10.1007/978-3-030-14223-0 External link